大桥未久AV手机在线观看

    1. <form id=sFmQbIMHg><nobr id=sFmQbIMHg></nobr></form>
      <address id=sFmQbIMHg><nobr id=sFmQbIMHg><nobr id=sFmQbIMHg></nobr></nobr></address>

      Source: New York Times
      Date: 10 January 2009

      My Genome, My Self

      By STEVEN PINKER

      ONE OF THE PERKS of being a psychologist is access to tools that allow you to carry out the injunction to know thyself. I have been tested for vocational interest (closest match: psychologist), intelligence (above average), personality (open, conscientious, agreeable, average in extraversion, not too neurotic) and political orientation (neither leftist nor rightist, more libertarian than authoritarian). I have M.R.I. pictures of my brain (no obvious holes or bulges) and soon will undergo the ultimate test of marital love: my brain will be scanned while my wife’s name is subliminally flashed before my eyes.

      Last fall I submitted to the latest high-tech way to bare your soul. I had my genome sequenced and am allowing it to be posted on the Internet, along with my medical history. The opportunity arose when the biologist George Church sought 10 volunteers to kick off his audacious Personal Genome Project. The P.G.P. has created a public database that will contain the genomes and traits of 100,000 people. Tapping the magic of crowd sourcing that gave us Wikipedia and Google rankings, the project seeks to engage geneticists in a worldwide effort to sift through the genetic and environmental predictors of medical, physical and behavioral traits.

      The Personal Genome Project is an initiative in basic research, not personal discovery. Yet the technological advance making it possible — the plunging cost of genome sequencing — will soon give people an unprecedented opportunity to contemplate their own biological and even psychological makeups. We have entered the era of consumer genetics. At one end of the price range you can get a complete sequence and analysis of your genome from Knome (often pronounced “know me”) for $99,500. At the other you can get a sample of traits, disease risks and ancestry data from 23andMe for $399. The science journal Nature listed “Personal Genomics Goes Mainstream” as a top news story of 2008.

      Like the early days of the Internet, the dawn of personal genomics promises benefits and pitfalls that no one can foresee. It could usher in an era of personalized medicine, in which drug regimens are customized for a patient’s biochemistry rather than juggled through trial and error, and screening and prevention measures are aimed at those who are most at risk. It opens up a niche for bottom-feeding companies to terrify hypochondriacs by turning dubious probabilities into Genes of Doom. Depending on who has access to the information, personal genomics could bring about national health insurance, leapfrogging decades of debate, because piecemeal insurance is not viable in a world in which insurers can cherry-pick the most risk-free customers, or in which at-risk customers can load up on lavish insurance.

      The pitfalls of personal genomics have already made it a subject of government attention. Last year President Bush signed the Genetic Information Nondiscrimination Act, outlawing discrimination in employment and health insurance based on genetic data. And the states of California and New York took action against the direct-to-consumer companies, arguing that what they provide are medical tests and thus can be ordered only by a doctor.

      With the genome no less than with the Internet, information wants to be free, and I doubt that paternalistic measures can stifle the industry for long (but then, I have a libertarian temperament). For better or for worse, people will want to know about their genomes. The human mind is prone to essentialism — the intuition that living things house some hidden substance that gives them their form and determines their powers. Over the past century, this essence has become increasingly concrete. Growing out of the early, vague idea that traits are “in the blood,” the essence became identified with the abstractions discovered by Gregor Mendel called genes, and then with the iconic double helix of DNA. But DNA has long been an invisible molecule accessible only to a white-coated priesthood. Today, for the price of a flat-screen TV, people can read their essence as a printout detailing their very own A’s, C’s, T’s and G’s.

      A firsthand familiarity with the code of life is bound to confront us with the emotional, moral and political baggage associated with the idea of our essential nature. People have long been familiar with tests for heritable diseases, and the use of genetics to trace ancestry — the new “Roots” — is becoming familiar as well. But we are only beginning to recognize that our genome also contains information about our temperaments and abilities. Affordable genotyping may offer new kinds of answers to the question “Who am I?” — to ruminations about our ancestry, our vulnerabilities, our character and our choices in life.

      Over the years I have come to appreciate how elusive the answers to those questions can be. During my first book tour 15 years ago, an interviewer noted that the paleontologist Stephen Jay Gould had dedicated his first book to his father, who took him to see the dinosaurs when he was 5. What was the event that made me become a cognitive psychologist who studies language? I was dumbstruck. The only thing that came to mind was that the human mind is uniquely interesting and that as soon as I learned you could study it for a living, I knew that that was what I wanted to do. But that response would not just have been charmless; it would also have failed to answer the question. Millions of people are exposed to cognitive psychology in college but have no interest in making a career of it. What made it so attractive to me?

      As I stared blankly, the interviewer suggested that perhaps it was because I grew up in Quebec in the 1970s when language, our pre-eminent cognitive capacity, figured so prominently in debates about the future of the province. I quickly agreed — and silently vowed to come up with something better for the next time. Now I say that my formative years were a time of raging debates about the political implications of human nature, or that my parents subscribed to a Time-Life series of science books, and my eye was caught by the one called “The Mind,” or that one day a friend took me to hear a lecture by the great Canadian psychologist D. O. Hebb, and I was hooked. But it is all humbug. The very fact that I had to think so hard brought home what scholars of autobiography and memoir have long recognized. None of us know what made us what we are, and when we have to say something, we make up a good story.

      An obvious candidate for the real answer is that we are shaped by our genes in ways that none of us can directly know. Of course genes can’t pull the levers of our behavior directly. But they affect the wiring and workings of the brain, and the brain is the seat of our drives, temperaments and patterns of thought. Each of us is dealt a unique hand of tastes and aptitudes, like curiosity, ambition, empathy, a thirst for novelty or for security, a comfort level with the social or the mechanical or the abstract. Some opportunities we come across click with our constitutions and set us along a path in life.

      This hardly seems radical — any parent of more than one child will tell you that babies come into the world with distinct personalities. But what can anyone say about how the baby got to be that way? Until recently, the only portents on offer were traits that ran in the family, and even they conflated genetic tendencies with family traditions. Now, at least in theory, personal genomics can offer a more precise explanation. We might be able to identify the actual genes that incline a person to being nasty or nice, an egghead or a doer, a sad sack or a blithe spirit.

      Looking to the genome for the nature of the person is far from innocuous. In the 20th century, many intellectuals embraced the idea that babies are blank slates that are inscribed by parents and society. It allowed them to distance themselves from toxic doctrines like that of a superior race, the eugenic breeding of a better species or a genetic version of the Twinkie Defense in which individuals or society could evade responsibility by saying that it’s all in the genes. When it came to human behavior, the attitude toward genetics was “Don’t go there.” Those who did go there found themselves picketed, tarred as Nazis and genetic determinists or, in the case of the biologist E. O. Wilson, doused with a pitcher of ice water at a scientific conference.

      Today, as the lessons of history have become clearer, the taboo is fading. Though the 20th century saw horrific genocides inspired by Nazi pseudoscience about genetics and race, it also saw horrific genocides inspired by Marxist pseudoscience about the malleability of human nature. The real threat to humanity comes from totalizing ideologies and the denial of human rights, rather than a curiosity about nature and nurture. Today it is the humane democracies of Scandinavia that are hotbeds of research in behavioral genetics, and two of the groups who were historically most victimized by racial pseudoscience — Jews and African-Americans — are among the most avid consumers of information about their genes.

      Nor should the scare word “determinism” get in the way of understanding our genetic roots. For some conditions, like Huntington’s disease, genetic determinism is simply correct: everyone with the defective gene who lives long enough will develop the condition. But for most other traits, any influence of the genes will be probabilistic. Having a version of a gene may change the odds, making you more or less likely to have a trait, all things being equal, but as we shall see, the actual outcome depends on a tangle of other circumstances as well.

      With personal genomics in its infancy, we can’t know whether it will deliver usable information about our psychological traits. But evidence from old-fashioned behavioral genetics — studies of twins, adoptees and other kinds of relatives — suggests that those genes are in there somewhere. Though once vilified as fraud-infested crypto-eugenics, behavioral genetics has accumulated sophisticated methodologies and replicable findings, which can tell us how much we can ever expect to learn about ourselves from personal genomics.

      To study something scientifically, you first have to measure it, and psychologists have developed tests for many mental traits. And contrary to popular opinion, the tests work pretty well: they give a similar measurement of a person every time they are administered, and they statistically predict life outcomes like school and job performance, psychiatric diagnoses and marital stability. Tests for intelligence might ask people to recite a string of digits backward, define a word like “predicament,” identify what an egg and a seed have in common or assemble four triangles into a square. Personality tests ask people to agree or disagree with statements like “Often I cross the street in order not to meet someone I know,” “I often was in trouble in school,” “Before I do something I try to consider how my friends will react to it” and “People say insulting and vulgar things about me.” People’s answers to a large set of these questions tend to vary in five major ways: openness to experience, conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness (as opposed to antagonism) and neuroticism. The scores can then be compared with those of relatives who vary in relatedness and family backgrounds.

      The most prominent finding of behavioral genetics has been summarized by the psychologist Eric Turkheimer: “The nature-nurture debate is over. . . . All human behavioral traits are heritable.” By this he meant that a substantial fraction of the variation among individuals within a culture can be linked to variation in their genes. Whether you measure intelligence or personality, religiosity or political orientation, television watching or cigarette smoking, the outcome is the same. Identical twins (who share all their genes) are more similar than fraternal twins (who share half their genes that vary among people). Biological siblings (who share half those genes too) are more similar than adopted siblings (who share no more genes than do strangers). And identical twins separated at birth and raised in different adoptive homes (who share their genes but not their environments) are uncannily similar.

      Behavioral geneticists like Turkheimer are quick to add that many of the differences among people cannot be attributed to their genes. First among these are the effects of culture, which cannot be measured by these studies because all the participants come from the same culture, typically middle-class European or American. The importance of culture is obvious from the study of history and anthropology. The reason that most of us don’t challenge each other to duels or worship our ancestors or chug down a nice warm glass of cow urine has nothing to do with genes and everything to do with the milieu in which we grew up. But this still leaves the question of why people in the same culture differ from one another.

      At this point behavioral geneticists will point to data showing that even within a single culture, individuals are shaped by their environments. This is another way of saying that a large fraction of the differences among individuals in any trait you care to measure do not correlate with differences among their genes. But a look at these nongenetic causes of our psychological differences shows that it’s far from clear what this “environment” is.

      Behavioral genetics has repeatedly found that the “shared environment” — everything that siblings growing up in the same home have in common, including their parents, their neighborhood, their home, their peer group and their school — has less of an influence on the way they turn out than their genes. In many studies, the shared environment has no measurable influence on the adult at all. Siblings reared together end up no more similar than siblings reared apart, and adoptive siblings reared in the same family end up not similar at all. A large chunk of the variation among people in intelligence and personality is not predictable from any obvious feature of the world of their childhood.

      Think of a pair of identical twins you know. They are probably highly similar, but they are certainly not indistinguishable. They clearly have their own personalities, and in some cases one twin can be gay and the other straight, or one schizophrenic and the other not. But where could these differences have come from? Not from their genes, which are identical. And not from their parents or siblings or neighborhood or school either, which were also, in most cases, identical. Behavioral geneticists attribute this mysterious variation to the “nonshared” or “unique” environment, but that is just a fudge factor introduced to make the numbers add up to 100 percent.

      No one knows what the nongenetic causes of individuality are. Perhaps people are shaped by modifications of genes that take place after conception, or by haphazard fluctuations in the chemical soup in the womb or the wiring up of the brain or the expression of the genes themselves. Even in the simplest organisms, genes are not turned on and off like clockwork but are subject to a lot of random noise, which is why genetically identical fruit flies bred in controlled laboratory conditions can end up with unpredictable differences in their anatomy. This genetic roulette must be even more significant in an organism as complex as a human, and it tells us that the two traditional shapers of a person, nature and nurture, must be augmented by a third one, brute chance.

      The discoveries of behavioral genetics call for another adjustment to our traditional conception of a nature-nurture cocktail. A common finding is that the effects of being brought up in a given family are sometimes detectable in childhood, but that they tend to peter out by the time the child has grown up. That is, the reach of the genes appears to get stronger as we age, not weaker. Perhaps our genes affect our environments, which in turn affect ourselves. Young children are at the mercy of parents and have to adapt to a world that is not of their choosing. As they get older, however, they can gravitate to the microenvironments that best suit their natures. Some children naturally lose themselves in the library or the local woods or the nearest computer; others ingratiate themselves with the jocks or the goths or the church youth group. Whatever genetic quirks incline a youth toward one niche or another will be magnified over time as they develop the parts of themselves that allow them to flourish in their chosen worlds. Also magnified are the accidents of life (catching or dropping a ball, acing or flubbing a test), which, according to the psychologist Judith Rich Harris, may help explain the seemingly random component of personality variation. The environment, then, is not a stamping machine that pounds us into a shape but a cafeteria of options from which our genes and our histories incline us to choose.

      All this sets the stage for what we can expect from personal genomics. Our genes are a big part of what we are. But even knowing the totality of genetic predictors, there will be many things about ourselves that no genome scan — and for that matter, no demographic checklist — will ever reveal. With these bookends in mind, I rolled up my sleeve, drooled into a couple of vials and awaited the results of three analyses of my DNA.

      The output of a complete genome scan would be a list of six billion A’s, C’s, G’s and T’s — a multigigabyte file that is still prohibitively expensive to generate and that, by itself, will always be perfectly useless. That is why most personal genomics ventures are starting with smaller portions of the genome that promise to contain nuggets of interpretable information.

      The Personal Genome Project is beginning with the exome: the 1 percent of our genome that is translated into strings of amino acids that assemble themselves into proteins. Proteins make up our physical structure, catalyze the chemical reactions that keep us alive and regulate the expression of other genes. The vast majority of heritable diseases that we currently understand involve tiny differences in one of the exons that collectively make up the exome, so it’s a logical place to start.

      Only a portion of my exome has been sequenced by the P.G.P. so far, none of it terribly interesting. But I did face a decision that will confront every genome consumer. Most genes linked to disease nudge the odds of developing the illness up or down a bit, and when the odds are increased, there is a recommended course of action, like more frequent testing or a preventive drug or a lifestyle change. But a few genes are perfect storms of bad news: high odds of developing a horrible condition that you can do nothing about. Huntington’s disease is one example, and many people whose family histories put them at risk (like Arlo Guthrie, whose father, Woody, died of the disease) choose not to learn whether they carry the gene.

      Another example is the apolipoprotein E gene (APOE). Nearly a quarter of the population carries one copy of the E4 variant, which triples their risk of developing Alzheimer’s disease. Two percent of people carry two copies of the gene (one from each parent), which increases their risk fifteenfold. James Watson, who with Francis Crick discovered the structure of DNA and who was one of the first two humans to have his genome sequenced, asked not to see which variant he had.

      As it turns out, we know what happens to people who do get the worst news. According to preliminary findings by the epidemiologist Robert C. Green, they don’t sink into despair or throw themselves off bridges; they handle it perfectly well. This should not be terribly surprising. All of us already live with the knowledge that we have the fatal genetic condition called mortality, and most of us cope using some combination of denial, resignation and religion. Still, I figured that my current burden of existential dread is just about right, so I followed Watson’s lead and asked for a line-item veto of my APOE gene information when the P.G.P. sequencer gets to it.

      The genes analyzed by a new company called Counsyl are more actionable, as they say in the trade. Their “universal carrier screen” is meant to tell prospective parents whether they carry genes that put their potential children at risk for more than a hundred serious diseases like cystic fibrosis and alpha thalassemia. If both parents have a copy of a recessive disease gene, there is a one-in-four chance that any child they conceive will develop the disease. With this knowledge they can choose to adopt a child instead or to undergo in-vitro fertilization and screen the embryos for the dangerous genes. It’s a scaled-up version of the Tay-Sachs test that Ashkenazi Jews have undergone for decades.

      I have known since 1972 that I am clean for Tay-Sachs, but the Counsyl screen showed that I carry one copy of a gene for familial dysautonomia, an incurable disorder of the autonomic nervous system that causes a number of unpleasant symptoms and a high chance of premature death. A well-meaning colleague tried to console me, but I was pleased to gain the knowledge. Children are not in my cards, but my nieces and nephews, who have a 25 percent chance of being carriers, will know to get tested. And I can shut the door to whatever wistfulness I may have had about my childlessness. The gene was not discovered until 2001, well after the choice confronted me, so my road not taken could have led to tragedy. But perhaps that’s the way you think if you are open to experience and not too neurotic.

      Familial dysautonomia is found almost exclusively among Ashkenazi Jews, and 23andMe provided additional clues to that ancestry in my genome. My mitochondrial DNA (which is passed intact from mother to offspring) is specific to Ashkenazi populations and is similar to ones found in Sephardic and Oriental Jews and in Druze and Kurds. My Y chromosome (which is passed intact from father to son) is also Levantine, common among Ashkenazi, Sephardic and Oriental Jews and also sprinkled across the eastern Mediterranean. Both variants arose in the Middle East more than 2,000 years ago and were probably carried to regions in Italy by Jewish exiles after the Roman destruction of Jerusalem, then to the Rhine Valley in the Middle Ages and eastward to the Pale of Settlement in Poland and Moldova, ending up in my father’s father and my mother’s mother a century ago.

      It’s thrilling to find yourself so tangibly connected to two millenniums of history. And even this secular, ecumenical Jew experienced a primitive tribal stirring in learning of a deep genealogy that coincides with the handing down of traditions I grew up with. But my blue eyes remind me not to get carried away with delusions about a Semitic essence. Mitochondrial DNA, and the Y chromosome, do not literally tell you about “your ancestry” but only half of your ancestry a generation ago, a quarter two generations ago and so on, shrinking exponentially the further back you go. In fact, since the further back you go the more ancestors you theoretically have (eight great-grandparents, sixteen great-great-grandparents and so on), at some point there aren’t enough ancestors to go around, everyone’s ancestors overlap with everyone else’s, and the very concept of personal ancestry becomes meaningless. I found it just as thrilling to zoom outward in the diagrams of my genetic lineage and see my place in a family tree that embraces all of humanity.

      As fascinating as carrier screening and ancestry are, the really new feature offered by 23andMe is its genetic report card. The company directs you to a Web page that displays risk factors for 14 diseases and 10 traits, and links to pages for an additional 51 diseases and 21 traits for which the scientific evidence is more iffy. Curious users can browse a list of markers from the rest of their genomes with a third-party program that searches a wiki of gene-trait associations that have been reported in the scientific literature. I found the site user-friendly and scientifically responsible. This clarity, though, made it easy to see that personal genomics has a long way to go before it will be a significant tool of self-discovery.

      The two biggest pieces of news I got about my disease risks were a 12.6 percent chance of getting prostate cancer before I turn 80 compared with the average risk for white men of 17.8 percent, and a 26.8 percent chance of getting Type 2 diabetes compared with the average risk of 21.9 percent. Most of the other outcomes involved even smaller departures from the norm. For a blessedly average person like me, it is completely unclear what to do with these odds. A one-in-four chance of developing diabetes should make any prudent person watch his weight and other risk factors. But then so should a one-in-five chance.

      It became all the more confusing when I browsed for genes beyond those on the summary page. Both the P.G.P. and the genome browser turned up studies that linked various of my genes to an elevated risk of prostate cancer, deflating my initial relief at the lowered risk. Assessing risks from genomic data is not like using a pregnancy-test kit with its bright blue line. It’s more like writing a term paper on a topic with a huge and chaotic research literature. You are whipsawed by contradictory studies with different sample sizes, ages, sexes, ethnicities, selection criteria and levels of statistical significance. Geneticists working for 23andMe sift through the journals and make their best judgments of which associations are solid. But these judgments are necessarily subjective, and they can quickly become obsolete now that cheap genotyping techniques have opened the floodgates to new studies.

      Direct-to-consumer companies are sometimes accused of peddling “recreational genetics,” and there’s no denying the horoscopelike fascination of learning about genes that predict your traits. Who wouldn’t be flattered to learn that he has two genes associated with higher I.Q. and one linked to a taste for novelty? It is also strangely validating to learn that I have genes for traits that I already know I have, like light skin and blue eyes. Then there are the genes for traits that seem plausible enough but make the wrong prediction about how I live my life, like my genes for tasting the bitterness in broccoli, beer and brussels sprouts (I consume them all), for lactose-intolerance (I seem to tolerate ice cream just fine) and for fast-twitch muscle fibers (I prefer hiking and cycling to basketball and squash). I also have genes that are nothing to brag about (like average memory performance and lower efficiency at learning from errors), ones whose meanings are a bit baffling (like a gene that gives me “typical odds” for having red hair, which I don’t have), and ones whose predictions are flat-out wrong (like a high risk of baldness).

      For all the narcissistic pleasure that comes from poring over clues to my inner makeup, I soon realized that I was using my knowledge of myself to make sense of the genetic readout, not the other way around. My novelty-seeking gene, for example, has been associated with a cluster of traits that includes impulsivity. But I don’t think I’m particularly impulsive, so I interpret the gene as the cause of my openness to experience. But then it may be like that baldness gene, and say nothing about me at all.

      Individual genes are just not very informative. Call it Geno’s Paradox. We know from classic medical and behavioral genetics that many physical and psychological traits are substantially heritable. But when scientists use the latest methods to fish for the responsible genes, the catch is paltry.

      Take height. Though health and nutrition can affect stature, height is highly heritable: no one thinks that Kareem Abdul-Jabbar just ate more Wheaties growing up than Danny DeVito. Height should therefore be a target-rich area in the search for genes, and in 2007 a genomewide scan of nearly 16,000 people turned up a dozen of them. But these genes collectively accounted for just 2 percent of the variation in height, and a person who had most of the genes was barely an inch taller, on average, than a person who had few of them. If that’s the best we can do for height, which can be assessed with a tape measure, what can we expect for more elusive traits like intelligence or personality?

      Geno’s Paradox entails that apart from carrier screening, personal genomics will be more recreational than diagnostic for some time to come. Some reasons are technological. The affordable genotyping services don’t actually sequence your entire genome but follow the time-honored scientific practice of looking for one’s keys under the lamppost because that’s where the light is best. They scan for half a million or so spots on the genome where a single nucleotide (half a rung on the DNA ladder) is likely to differ from one person to the next. These differences are called Single Nucleotide Polymorphisms, or SNPs (pronounced “snips”), and they can be cheaply identified en masse by putting a dollop of someone’s DNA on a device called a microarray or SNP chip. A SNP can be a variant of a gene, or can serve as a signpost for variants of a gene that are nearby.

      But not all genetic variation comes in the form of these one-letter typos. A much larger portion of our genomes varies in other ways. A chunk of DNA may be missing or inverted or duplicated, or a tiny substring may be repeated different numbers of times — say, five times in one person and seven times in another. These variations are known to cause diseases and differences in personality, but unless they accompany a particular SNP, they will not turn up on a SNP chip.

      As sequencing technology improves, more of our genomic variations will come into view. But determining what those variants mean is another matter. A good day for geneticists is one in which they look for genes that have nice big effects and that are found in many people. But remember the minuscule influence of each of the genes that affects stature. There may be hundreds of other such genes, each affecting height by an even smaller smidgen, but it is hard to discern the genes in this long tail of the distribution amid the cacophony of the entire genome. And so it may be for the hundreds or thousands of genes that make you a teensy bit smarter or duller, calmer or more jittery.

      Another kind of headache for geneticists comes from gene variants that do have large effects but that are unique to you or to some tiny fraction of humanity. These, too, are hard to spot in genomewide scans. Say you have a unique genetic variant that gives you big ears. The problem is that you have other unique genes as well. Since it would be literally impossible to assemble a large sample of people who do and don’t have the crucial gene and who do and don’t have big ears, there is no way to know which of your proprietary genes is the culprit. If we understood the molecular assembly line by which ears were put together in the embryo, we could identify the gene by what it does rather than by what it correlates with. But with most traits, that’s not yet possible — not for ears, and certainly not for a sense of humor or a gift of gab or a sweet disposition. In fact, the road to discovery in biology often goes in the other direction. Biologists discover the genetic pathways that build an organ by spotting genes that correlate with different forms of it and then seeing what they do.

      So how likely is it that future upgrades to consumer genomics kits will turn up markers for psychological traits? The answer depends on why we vary in the first place, an unsolved problem in behavioral genetics. And the answer may be different for different psychological traits.

      In theory, we should hardly differ at all. Natural selection works like compound interest: a gene with even a 1 percent advantage in the number of surviving offspring it yields will expand geometrically over a few hundred generations and quickly crowd out its less fecund alternatives. Why didn’t this winnowing leave each of us with the best version of every gene, making each of us as vigorous, smart and well adjusted as human physiology allows? The world would be a duller place, but evolution doesn’t go out of its way to keep us entertained.

      It’s tempting to say that society as a whole prospers with a mixture of tinkers, tailors, soldiers, sailors and so on. But evolution selects among genes, not societies, and if the genes that make tinkers outreproduce the genes that make tailors, the tinker genes will become a monopoly. A better way of thinking about genetic diversity is that if everyone were a tinker, it would pay to have tailor genes, and the tailor genes would start to make an inroad, but then as society filled up with tailor genes, the advantage would shift back to the tinkers. A result would be an equilibrium with a certain proportion of tinkers and a certain proportion of tailors. Biologists call this process balancing selection: two designs for an organism are equally fit, but in different physical or social environments, including the environments that consist of other members of the species. Often the choice between versions of such a trait is governed by a single gene, or a few adjacent genes that are inherited together. If instead the trait were controlled by many genes, then during sexual reproduction those genes would get all mixed up with the genes from the other parent, who might have the alternative version of the trait. Over several generations the genes for the two designs would be thoroughly scrambled, and the species would be homogenized.

      The psychologists Lars Penke, Jaap Denissen and Geoffrey Miller argue that personality differences arise from this process of balancing selection. Selfish people prosper in a world of nice guys, until they become so common that they start to swindle one another, whereupon nice guys who cooperate get the upper hand, until there are enough of them for the swindlers to exploit, and so on. The same balancing act can favor rebels in a world of conformists and vice-versa, or doves in a world of hawks.

      The optimal personality may also depend on the opportunities and risks presented by different environments. The early bird gets the worm, but the second mouse gets the cheese. An environment that has worms in some parts but mousetraps in others could select for a mixture of go-getters and nervous nellies. More plausibly, it selects for organisms that sniff out what kind of environment they are in and tune their boldness accordingly, with different individuals setting their danger threshold at different points.

      But not all variation in nature arises from balancing selection. The other reason that genetic variation can persist is that rust never sleeps: new mutations creep into the genome faster than natural selection can weed them out. At any given moment, the population is laden with a portfolio of recent mutations, each of whose days are numbered. This Sisyphean struggle between selection and mutation is common with traits that depend on many genes, because there are so many things that can go wrong.

      Penke, Denissen and Miller argue that a mutation-selection standoff is the explanation for why we differ in intelligence. Unlike personality, where it takes all kinds to make a world, with intelligence, smarter is simply better, so balancing selection is unlikely. But intelligence depends on a large network of brain areas, and it thrives in a body that is properly nourished and free of diseases and defects. Many genes are engaged in keeping this system going, and so there are many genes that, when mutated, can make us a little bit stupider.

      At the same time there aren’t many mutations that can make us a whole lot smarter. Mutations in general are far more likely to be harmful than helpful, and the large, helpful ones were low-hanging fruit that were picked long ago in our evolutionary history and entrenched in the species. One reason for this can be explained with an analogy inspired by the mathematician Ronald Fisher. A large twist of a focusing knob has some chance of bringing a microscope into better focus when it is far from the best setting. But as the barrel gets closer to the target, smaller and smaller tweaks are needed to bring any further improvement.

      The Penke/Denissen/Miller theory, which attributes variation in personality and intelligence to different evolutionary processes, is consistent with what we have learned so far about the genes for those two kinds of traits. The search for I.Q. genes calls to mind the cartoon in which a scientist with a smoldering test tube asks a colleague, “What’s the opposite of Eureka?” Though we know that genes for intelligence must exist, each is likely to be small in effect, found in only a few people, or both. In a recent study of 6,000 children, the gene with the biggest effect accounted for less than one-quarter of an I.Q. point. The quest for genes that underlie major disorders of cognition, like autism and schizophrenia, has been almost as frustrating. Both conditions are highly heritable, yet no one has identified genes that cause either condition across a wide range of people. Perhaps this is what we should expect for a high-maintenance trait like human cognition, which is vulnerable to many mutations.

      The hunt for personality genes, though not yet Nobel-worthy, has had better fortunes. Several associations have been found between personality traits and genes that govern the breakdown, recycling or detection of neurotransmitters (the molecules that seep from neuron to neuron) in the brain systems underlying mood and motivation.

      Dopamine is the molecular currency in several brain circuits associated with wanting, getting satisfaction and paying attention. The gene for one kind of dopamine receptor, DRD4, comes in several versions. Some of the variants (like the one I have) have been associated with “approach related” personality traits like novelty seeking, sensation seeking and extraversion. A gene for another kind of receptor, DRD2, comes in a version that makes its dopamine system function less effectively. It has been associated with impulsivity, obesity and substance abuse. Still another gene, COMT, produces an enzyme that breaks down dopamine in the prefrontal cortex, the home of higher cognitive functions like reasoning and planning. If your version of the gene produces less COMT, you may have better concentration but might also be more neurotic and jittery.

      Behavioral geneticists have also trained their sights on serotonin, which is found in brain circuits that affect many moods and drives, including those affected by Prozac and similar drugs. SERT, the serotonin transporter, is a molecule that scoops up stray serotonin for recycling, reducing the amount available to act in the brain. The switch for the gene that makes SERT comes in long and short versions, and the short version has been linked to depression and anxiety. A 2003 study made headlines because it suggested that the gene may affect a person’s resilience to life’s stressors rather than giving them a tendency to be depressed or content across the board. People who had two short versions of the gene (one from each parent) were likely to have a major depressive episode only if they had undergone traumatic experiences; those who had a more placid history were fine. In contrast, people who had two long versions of the gene typically failed to report depression regardless of their life histories. In other words, the effects of the gene are sensitive to a person’s environment. Psychologists have long known that some people are resilient to life’s slings and arrows and others are more fragile, but they had never seen this interaction played out in the effects of individual genes.

      Still other genes have been associated with trust and commitment, or with a tendency to antisocial outbursts. It’s still a messy science, with plenty of false alarms, contradictory results and tiny effects. But consumers will probably learn of genes linked to personality before they see any that are reliably connected to intelligence.

      Personal genomics is here to stay. The science will improve as efforts like the Personal Genome Project amass huge samples, the price of sequencing sinks and biologists come to a better understanding of what genes do and why they vary. People who have grown up with the democratization of information will not tolerate paternalistic regulations that keep them from their own genomes, and early adopters will explore how this new information can best be used to manage our health. There are risks of misunderstandings, but there are also risks in much of the flimflam we tolerate in alternative medicine, and in the hunches and folklore that many doctors prefer to evidence-based medicine. And besides, personal genomics is just too much fun.

      At the same time, there is nothing like perusing your genetic data to drive home its limitations as a source of insight into yourself. What should I make of the nonsensical news that I am “probably light-skinned” but have a “twofold risk of baldness”? These diagnoses, of course, are simply peeled off the data in a study: 40 percent of men with the C version of the rs2180439 SNP are bald, compared with 80 percent of men with the T version, and I have the T. But something strange happens when you take a number representing the proportion of people in a sample and apply it to a single individual. The first use of the number is perfectly respectable as an input into a policy that will optimize the costs and benefits of treating a large similar group in a particular way. But the second use of the number is just plain weird. Anyone who knows me can confirm that I’m not 80 percent bald, or even 80 percent likely to be bald; I’m 100 percent likely not to be bald. The most charitable interpretation of the number when applied to me is, “If you knew nothing else about me, your subjective confidence that I am bald, on a scale of 0 to 10, should be 8.” But that is a statement about your mental state, not my physical one. If you learned more clues about me (like seeing photographs of my father and grandfathers), that number would change, while not a hair on my head would be different. Some mathematicians say that “the probability of a single event” is a meaningless concept.

      Even when the effect of some gene is indubitable, the sheer complexity of the self will mean that it will not serve as an oracle on what the person will do. The gene that lets me taste propyl?thiouracil, 23andMe suggests, might make me dislike tonic water, coffee and dark beer. Unlike the tenuous genes linked to personality or intelligence, this one codes for a single taste-bud receptor, and I don’t doubt that it lets me taste the bitterness. So why hasn’t it stopped me from enjoying those drinks? Presumably it’s because adults get a sophisticated pleasure from administering controlled doses of aversive stimuli to themselves. I’ve acquired a taste for Beck’s Dark; others enjoy saunas, rock-climbing, thrillers or dissonant music. Similarly, why don’t I conform to type and exploit those fast-twitch muscle fibers (thanks, ACTN3 genes!) in squash or basketball, rather than wasting them on hiking? A lack of coordination, a love of the outdoors, an inclination to daydream, all of the above? The self is a byzantine bureaucracy, and no gene can push the buttons of behavior by itself. You can attribute the ability to defy our genotypes to free will, whatever that means, but you can also attribute it to the fact that in a hundred-trillion-synapse human brain, any single influence can be outweighed by the product of all of the others.

      Even if personal genomics someday delivers a detailed printout of psychological traits, it will probably not change everything, or even most things. It will give us deeper insight about the biological causes of individuality, and it may narrow the guesswork in assessing individual cases. But the issues about self and society that it brings into focus have always been with us. We have always known that people are liable, to varying degrees, to antisocial temptations and weakness of the will. We have always known that people should be encouraged to develop the parts of themselves that they can (“a man’s reach should exceed his grasp”) but that it’s foolish to expect that anyone can accomplish anything (“a man has got to know his limitations”). And we know that holding people responsible for their behavior will make it more likely that they behave responsibly. “My genes made me do it” is no better an excuse than “We’re depraved on account of we’re deprived.”

      Many of the dystopian fears raised by personal genomics are simply out of touch with the complex and probabilistic nature of genes. Forget about the hyperparents who want to implant math genes in their unborn children, the “Gattaca” corporations that scan people’s DNA to assign them to castes, the employers or suitors who hack into your genome to find out what kind of worker or spouse you’d make. Let them try; they’d be wasting their time.

      The real-life examples are almost as futile. When the connection between the ACTN3 gene and muscle type was discovered, parents and coaches started swabbing the cheeks of children so they could steer the ones with the fast-twitch variant into sprinting and football. Carl Foster, one of the scientists who uncovered the association, had a better idea: “Just line them up with their classmates for a race and see which ones are the fastest.” Good advice. The test for a gene can identify one of the contributors to a trait. A measurement of the trait itself will identify all of them: the other genes (many or few, discovered or undiscovered, understood or not understood), the way they interact, the effects of the environment and the child’s unique history of developmental quirks.

      It’s our essentialist mind-set that makes the cheek swab feel as if it is somehow a deeper, truer, more authentic test of the child’s ability. It’s not that the mind-set is utterly misguided. Our genomes truly are a fundamental part of us. They are what make us human, including the distinctively human ability to learn and create culture. They account for at least half of what makes us different from our neighbors. And though we can change both inherited and acquired traits, changing the inherited ones is usually harder. It is a question of the most perspicuous level of analysis at which to understand a complex phenomenon. You can’t understand the stock market by studying a single trader, or a movie by putting a DVD under a microscope. The fallacy is not in thinking that the entire genome matters, but in thinking that an individual gene will matter, at least in a way that is large and intelligible enough for us to care about.

      So if you are bitten by scientific or personal curiosity and can think in probabilities, by all means enjoy the fruits of personal genomics. But if you want to know whether you are at risk for high cholesterol, have your cholesterol measured; if you want to know whether you are good at math, take a math test. And if you really want to know yourself (and this will be the test of how much you do), consider the suggestion of Fran?ois La Rochefoucauld: “Our enemies’ opinion of us comes closer to the truth than our own.”

      Steven Pinker is Harvard College professor of psychology at Harvard University and the author of “The Stuff of Thought: Language as a Window Into Human Nature.”


      The Reproductive Revolution
      Refs
      and further reading

      HOME
      Resources
      BLTC Research
      Liberal Eugenics
      Superhappiness?
      Utopian Surgery?
      The End of Suffering
      Wirehead Hedonism
      The Good Drug Guide
      The Abolitionist Project
      The Hedonistic Imperative
      The Reproductive Revolution
      MDMA: Utopian Pharmacology
      Transhumanism: Brave New World?
      Critique of Aldous Huxley's Brave New World

      e-mail
      dave@bltc.com

      "'I'll make old vases for you if you want them—will make them just as I made these.' He had visions of a room full of golden brown beard. It was the most appalling thing he had ever witnessed, and there was no trickery about it. The beard had actually grown before his eyes, and it had now reached to the second button of the Clockwork man's waistcoat. And, at any moment, Mrs. Masters might return! "Worth stealing," a Society journalist lounging by remarked. "I could write a novel, only I can never think of a plot. Your old housekeeper is asleep long ago. Where do you carry your latchkey?" "Never lose your temper," he said. "It leads to apoplexy. Ah, my fine madam, you thought to pinch me, but I have pinched you instead." How does that strike you, Mr. Smith? Fancy Jerusha Abbott, (individually) ever pat me on the head, Daddy? I don't believe so-- The confusion was partly inherited from Aristotle. When discussing the psychology of that philosopher, we showed that his active Nous is no other than the idea of which we are at any moment actually conscious. Our own reason is the passive Nous, whose identity is lost in the multiplicity of objects with which it becomes identified in turn. But Aristotle was careful not to let the personality of God, or the supreme Nous, be endangered by resolving it into the totality of substantial forms which constitute Nature. God is self-conscious in the strictest sense. He thinks nothing but himself. Again, the subjective starting-point of305 Plotinus may have affected his conception of the universal Nous. A single individual may isolate himself from his fellows in so far as he is a sentient being; he cannot do so in so far as he is a rational being. His reason always addresses itself to the reason of some one else—a fact nowhere brought out so clearly as in the dialectic philosophy of Socrates and Plato. Then, when an agreement has been established, their minds, before so sharply divided, seem to be, after all, only different personifications of the same universal spirit. Hence reason, no less than its objects, comes to be conceived as both many and one. And this synthesis of contradictories meets us in modern German as well as in ancient Greek philosophy. 216 "I shall be mighty glad when we git this outfit to Chattanoogy," sighed Si. "I'm gittin' older every minute that I have 'em on my hands." "What was his name?" inquired Monty Scruggs. "Wot's worth while?" "Rose, Rose—my dear, my liddle dear—you d?an't mean——" "I'm out of practice, or I shouldn't have skinned myself like this—ah, here's Coalbran's trap. Perhaps he'll give you a lift, ma'am, into Peasmarsh." Chapter 18 "The Fair-pl?ace." "Yes," replied Black Jack, "here they are," drawing a parchment from his pocket. "This is the handwriting of a retainer called Oakley." HoME大桥未久AV手机在线观看 ENTER NUMBET 0016www.ho84.org.cn
      www.l3tbb.net.cn
      www.wangcio.com.cn
      tarland.com.cn
      ukerd.net.cn
      thrjn.com.cn
      www.uhfjwz.com.cn
      www.weiyigo.com.cn
      weidetiyu.net.cn
      www.xfde.com.cn
      丝袜美腿恋足和脚交全部成人电影电驴下载 簧色图片狠簧狠狠秃噜 美熟母快播电影网 伊斯兰国妇女性交电影 少妇喜欢被操吗 留守熟娘自拍 国产全裸写真下载地址 操逼小说之母女 波多野结衣专辑 淫乱家庭日 另类图片天天 oumeisetuhahasedouhang 欧美丝袜厕所脚 学生妹内射图 WWW_68ABAB_COM 大奶裸体美乳 此影片受美国法律堡在线 日本爱视频 少爷 雏 开苞 偷拍孕妇的乳房 第一部家庭淫乱史 mgscl55 avsiwa 最新免费艺术片 俩性与爱图删除 bt 欧美 肛交 小77婷婷五月天 淫荡的少妇快播 男人狂插女人狂叫 屄【p】 陈静仪乳头很大吗 西西人体穴 qiangjianxiaoyima 藤井蕾娜 欧美乱伦交 狠狠抠女人 五月天逼逼图片 美女写真有哪些 丝袜漏在线播放 骚女浪妇乱伦爱爱 张芳人体艺术摄影 成人女孩网 台湾插18人体艺术图 高清无码人体艺术的网站 男女同事肏屄的小说 丁香熟女少妇乱伦图 浙江真实乱伦迅雷 天空中出的170cm电梯小姐种子 母子父女操逼乱伦小说或偷拍 美国十次啦毛片 淫荡二嫂 熟女人人妻18p 国产十大美女3在线播放 南京师范大学菁林园 尾行3打不开 裸体照下面带毛 导航少妇 哥哥就要操 父女交欢 女性人体黄照视频 zhan昭艳谈 羽田爱影音先锋 女人阴道屁毛裸体全身照片一一 狂草美人3p 日屄性爱激情 wohelilaoshixingjiao 女人外阴实物图真人 d人体艺术网 母子交合怀孕的小说 辰溪四中艳照门照片 国外男性美图 姨妈美丽和乱伦 模特羽田人体 古色古香 丁字裤小穴 亚洲图片欧美图片色妹妹 我爱仓井空 自拍亚洲欧美迅雷下载电驴下载 国产偷拍论坛下载 史上最大性器bt 14岁屁眼 dzu7788 日本做爱刑片 欧美超性感高跟丝袜艳舞 紫涵影音先锋av 世界操骚逼新闻 操熊黛林小说 欧美黑丝老师做爱 和妈妈一家做爱 中国女人屄屄图片 黑人干美女的的电影 韩国大奶少妇诱惑图 乱伦少妇系列小说 日本人体艺术乳秀 德国黄色片男女偷情同性恋强奸 我和妈妈 91tv影院软件下载 WWW55ABCDECOM 印尼屠华qvod 少妇大胆鲍鱼激情成人小说快播 黑丝网妇在线 谁有在线看g片网站 WWW520MSNCOM 哥哥舔我文 零久少女秘宝馆成人套图 凌辱丝袜美女 国内莫航空公司空姐性爱视频合集影音先锋 三级片免费在线网站 西西人体葵司 曹颖欧美丰满女人大逼人体艺术 家庭乱伦97ai 天天干夜撸 幼女13岁女睡后毛才五六根rmbttd 日本无码qvod 小明看看平台se7se7 国产自拍少妇内射在线 肉色丝袜脚超清 兽交网址很插美女穴亚洲色 近亲相奸影院 老婆怀孕我和岳母做爱 身上光溜溜的女人图片 女人阴口是屁股眼吗 弟弟插妹妹的故事 搜库曹查理 欧美大胆写真种子视频 911sss911s 色狼漫画人鱼 黑丝高跟影速先锋 五月天激情网迅雷下载 12岁女孩逼什么样 欧洲美女冶剖悠 熟女无料h动画 骚穴yang 母亲儿子做爱乱伦电影 涩皮娘子滑皮官 丰满大白屁股 衅夹大逼松 欧美性爱高清快播 日韩近亲先锋影音 琪琪影院经典片 亚洲少女操逼图 色干女 处女第一次性交视频合集 妻子让朋友干出尿 莫色一片段 强奸萝莉的h小说 wwwzxulicom 亚洲色图p伊人 山村母女抽B 最新国产自拍高潮合集迅雷 bbS师城网影视大全大全性交 捆绑美女老师 和妈妈的乱伦性爱小说岳母 hentai天天酷跑 新新电影理论人妻 qyuletv青娱乐在线 丰满大屁股嫂子 影音先锋ady三级片 setu22进入中 阿姨叫从后面进入 av动画优优色影院 亚洲色图久草网偷拍自拍视频 ww51zhaofupo官网 短篇黄书txt下载 开女儿嫩苞的小说 空天使亚洲色图 吹箫三级片古代 ppt播放avi黑屏 留学生性爱偷拍视频 武侠古典潘金莲 大陆色屌操 duopapadiz 牛d叉电影伦理电影 搜索www人与兽交 黄色电影三级片老毛女 大奶妹激吻动态图 sese尤物 亚洲强奸丝袜文章 舔B乳头 三级网络地址直接免费五月天 那个文字magnet kp快播成人版 av色老太老头 美女AV视频xw970com 老师干儿子淫秽 成人流氓视频下载 春药火腿肠 网友ed2k 美女图片的逼逼屁屁前边 超碰淫淫网 gan38comwwwgzyunhecom 农夫色资源 SM真实自拍 邻家阿姨叫我添她下面 激情五夜四射 八戒街拍裙底 wwwluotinuren 亚洲色图网站激情套图 阜阳GAY 人妻淫淫网 久久外国三级视频 吖吖色资源吧 纵犬潜伏讯雷 明星淫乱狠狠 东方在线出行 久久电影网wwwdy99cn b33mmminfo 日本人与狗种子 亚洲电影二区 大黑逼25 huangse淫淫wang 大胆人体摸特乱轮小说 三级片最狠的是什么 免费淫图 张筱雨人体身影下载 苍井空操逼时候视频 巨乳的诱惑电影 sao妹妹电影 我为明日花死 AV制服丝袜 13yn_com 公交车上被啊啊啊好爽 俺去也先锋小说 海贼王色色视频手机在线观看 CADV480 幼激情社区 影音先锋淫荡人妻 av大帝骚色在线视频 A片性交电影 四虎影库必出精品浅仓彩音 ooxx名侦探柯南 色春阁熟女18p 成人王网站 最新激情图片 欧美sm暴力虐待女图片 自拍av射射 免XX网址 欧美性爱偷拍自拍图片 女人下部真实图片 三级a片在线 动漫女生的恶搞图片 9sekecim 色艳片 一群女人靠逼 幼幼导航最干净的 淫媳荡翁的性生活爱爱 偷拍自拍第1页大香蕉 日日撸成人 老师夹得我好紧 国内自拍强奸乱伦 www4d4d4dm 一本美女性交生活 2017最新流出视频种子 青青草影院 熟女诱惑微拍 日韩制服丝片 亚洲色视频无码专区 gv视频在线免费观看 做爱鸡巴疼怎么办啊 姐妹监禁教师 父女乱伦视频网站 小纯洁777在线影院 影音先锋欧美性爱AV 人体艺术野战 琪琪网站色猫网 t多毛中文学院视频 日妣漫画 下载激 Hh网 蝌蚪窝vr pin6biz 淘宝团购 人人操色8 anquse 捆绑强奸娇喘视频 偷窥自拍欧美色图在线播放 成人电影快播影院 色老网 Kjfulic0m bt种子下载成人无码 人体艺术偷拍另类 亚洲色图区大色小色 wwwf4yycon 幼10P 偷拍自拍另类贴图 3344nq zz1818 htppwwwbbbvodcom haodiaoorg的综合查询 062bcomwap231dycom 少妇诱惑舞蹈 西西国模 JQWY5COM 淫浪网 就爱草逼逼 在线观看田中美佐野战 adyavz r3影院 亚裔成人色图 52av若怒 gegean 触手录官网 阿片吧 萝莉另类小说 wwwxxx美少女 豆豆rrcom 在线AV偷拍视频 草榴掰阴偷拍自拍 人体艺术秀美女贴图 日本大奶老熟女肥阴 金发天国在线播放1 狠狠撸不需下载播放器的av片 刘亦菲不雅动态图 动漫av迅雷下载 免费手机av皇帝 偷拍自拍野外母子做爱 iuo体图片美女gu乳房 色和尚香蕉撸 性爱技巧校园春色小说 WWW_H222_COMHWWW 啊啊啊av 爱幼幼社区 mkkavtv 音影先锋大香蕉伊人 第九影院第一页 99re6久久热在线观看3 肉体ys 日本猜人游戏在线影院 黑丝袜的逼 美国小孩性交配 wwwrrrfffcom 9977玖玖 范冰冰狠狠撸种子 3wbbbcom 网yiny 男人吃女人的乳房好吗 人妻好吊视频在线观看 色在线奶奶 8vaa褋芯屑 七七电影第一页 snn妻妻彬 MXGS673 粉嫩人体美穴 网吧嗯啊 天天撸撸丁香 88街拍视频网 欧美小萝莉6080 快播伦理艺术 第二色激情 av一本道视频在线观看 尼姑福利网 www路993bb路com 偷拍自拍新疆妹子 偷拍自拍福利都市 另类小说迅雷专区 给老婆找鸭子magnet 超碰yjizztv 手机在线你懂的免费 厕沟间谍spychinaclu 922bbb966bbb肛交 丰满骚妇淫叫 wwwky757com为什么看不了 44kkmmtvcom 久久精品在线5 性虐黄色电影 五月天色哥哥色妹妹 狠狠干狠狠做狠狠爱 一本道无玛avwwwvid11comwwwvid11com 日本金山升一在线视频 求在线能看黄色的网站 久久色女儿阁 国模大胆阴部 国产操逼A片 亚洲无码凸凹视频 黄色视频啪啪啪网站在线观看 黄色艺术乱论故事 强奸乱伦8爱色电影院 打飞机时看的裸照 国模图片小说三级图片 插B十V是啥意思 中国老奶奶的毛wwwredtubecom 伦理偷拍自拍人妻少妇 林志玲rentiyishu 新疆男人鸡吧图片 九九热翘臀 在线播放波多野结衣A级视频 www295555ocm 醉地撸成人免费av视频 412vvcom 干了五个美女的小屁眼 男人资源高清无码 美女人体艺术pp胸 偷拍大学生美术课上裸体写真图 蔡美玲 ed2k 快播成人之美连续剧 日本中学生乳艺术 李宗瑞迷奸门快播全集 操嫂嫂b文章 eva美女邪恶漫画 撸师妹亚洲色图 和邻居姐姐做爱 小说我和小姨性生活 大鸡吧色图 求东京热网盘mp4 濑穴 骚女有种子 绑起来干33p 肥女人体艺照 国模林娜 913p 无美美鲍人体艺术 国外幼幼乱伦电影 泰国佬猛插日本性娘3p大战 丝袜 熟女 欧美 韩国性感美女操逼视频 国模杜箐箐私处 WWW_DUOTE_COMTECHK 欧美成人激情综合 熟妇口交照片 爱爱谷商城射黑丝袜 大鸡巴日大奶美女骚屄图片 日本人体七艺术 先锋成人乱伦强暴网址 国产操少妇 父女性爱母子性交 av 色 苍井空搞一次多少钱 日本无码电影qvod 好莱坞女星照片泄漏下载百度云 wo de laoshishi 骚逼 小77乱伦图片 激情日本人体艺术 女生 肥逼大奶老妇女全祼激情图 45chengren d9e4590e0000131d 黑木明纱无码快播 美国美女十次啦 曰本艺术图片 男女真人噪b写真 就去写真 看明星簧片的 亿性家自拍偷拍视频 自拍偷拍激情艳照 小乳房嫩比美女 乱搞性爱生活 扶苏 狱锁狂龙5 李宗瑞视频在那 激情亚洲色妹妹奇米 好色妻降临美兰惠子 美国人家庭性爱乱伦春暖花开 我爱插逼网色图 美女与黑人操逼图贴 daruoxue 美女性爱过程图 白洁骚屄有声小说 黑黑色成人免费视频 伊一人体合成图 电影明星操逼 超级人体艺术p10 黄色电子书色播 谁有少妇图片 xxx4tubetv 痷去也图区 奈香里子 莎丽人体 波多野结衣美图作品集 哥哥AV网视 丝袜骚妇人妻乱伦 亚州色图27p 欧美丝袜就去色色 渡涞晶人体艺术 强奸在线成人视频 丝袜播放器 韩国美人图未来影院神马电影 教师操屄 明被迷奸 苍井空qvod电影在线 天天激综合总站 色男色女3级片 射人阁我要爱爱网站 看电影来5566有闲夫人 f05bbd3e00007510 超嫩10岁妹子嫩穴嫩肉嫩奶子 ah442百度影院 体恐惧内射玉i农民精过2 人体艺体祼体 午夜影院播放版破解版下载 RE789COM mp3黄片一级 torrent日本av 欧美性爱大图色图强奸乱 h小游戏口交 网友自拍草榴裙社区 无码动画大全 激情3p图 苍井空爆操女人前后 逍遥社区淫淫就去色色 爱爱美少女成人网 WWW34SPZCOM 幼幼资源影音先锋 女人的阴毛无马赛克 WWWXINAISHEDESCOM 男同番号 中文sexxxx 影音先锋可播的成人动漫 大胆高清美女人体艺术图片图 色图色噜 动态男女性交图片 老年夫妻操逼视频 激情网站wwwhuangsedianyingnet luluhei网站最新地址 久操视频偷拍 亚洲图片欧美偷拍o 乱乱色图 淫欲女秘书在线观看 狐狸精导航 卡通动漫欧美日本 嫖娼影音先锋资源 性交动攸 Av天天操 黄色舔逼逼小说 摸骚妇宾馆 肛交av网站 被轮奸的感觉真爽 mmp4ppC0m 好屌色公开视觉 大香蕉网人人摸 户外露出的网站 奇奇热奇奇色妻子撸 大学生情趣网袜 guaishou521 干干爽 亚洲是图p1百度臊妇淫荡图片 手机Avzx 青青草www99kk496cn 久久在线视频精品99re6mweibocn 操逼的牛牛 黄色特级aaaaa片 激情女同小说另类 欧洲人做太国在线Av 胖哥人体艺术 赵东赫 插妹妹黄色网站 美女口交舔逼小说 久久幼女孩色视频 成人黄色电影国产片 欧美激情淫荡熟妇 88tv熟女人妻 小色网视频女友想要 123首播伦理影院 爱人人体艺术摄影裸体照 少妇的qq或微信 影音资源激情小说家庭乱伦 出租车抠逼 流出淫水 2017搜片神器是那个 气密四色 调教奸肉棒性奴 快播热女人妻小说 青青草少女学生 少妇自拍偷拍在线111kk 色天使在钱 亚洲色图欧美另类校园春色 爱撸吧tumblr 四房播播丁香区 九九版国产黑人操免费视频 北岛玲操逼图 吉吉资源变态 操美女美穴 se情wuyue天 肌肉女做爱英文 色哥哥无码性爱电影 黄色直播网 91gav永久网址 丁香花五月婷婷开之人与兽心 在线大鸡吧干小骚货翻白眼小说 美女做爱亲热图 染岛贡强奸 春色天乃水 樱井莉亚人 求手机黄网啊 色五月开心五月五月天 东京热多少集 东京热0108 不错东京热 俺去啦酒色网 黄色小说酒色网 javhd 长泽あずさ oumeixingai 妈妈与儿子16p 就爱母亲 小骚女影视 家庭乱伦A片 五月天123 哥也撸 婷婷网 乐途旅游网 爱情动作片网站你懂的 哥也要 爱我久久 grch系列在哪有 3344wv亚洲 啪啪啪漫画书网址 又黄又色裸美女影院 能考A片的网站 桃花色热热色综合网 谷露影院日韩 动态图片亚洲在线 色和尚久久爱久久瑟 snis-937在线 伊凡综合成人 gav成人免安装播放 2018秋霞理论电网在线视频 特殊任务 葵 国语爱爱迅雷 magnet 啪啪啪激情黄片视频 情人节快乐视频大奶子女人是谁 秋霞理伦片 下载 人妻熟女婷婷开心影院 羞羞影院每日黄片 群交影院yy834 小学女生三级湿影院 日本情视频高清在线观看讨厌马赛克 日日看超碰在线91 影音先锋乱伦小说 日韩 网红 福利 自拍 另类 又幼穴 樱井莉亚菊门 淫色人妻老司机在线播放 强行占有美女 AV日本电影 湿湿影院网址 在线小萝莉 教师妈妈A片 天昊影院理论片 欧美人兽操逼 巨乳少妇视屏在线 谭干聪 优京香 香澄遥美人女教师 热的 综合 另类 吹潮 抖阴国际版成人电影 久久鸭AV色和尚 涂油av 色噜噜插 云拇指智能视频 A级片i子片 1四虎影剧在线播放 51涩涩 高清国产自拍 在线视频 小清影院AV 999国产厕所偷拍视频 一本道视频穴 国产偷拍主题宾馆在线 被大鸡吧塞满的视频 能看到美女阴唇的视频 淫荡少妇乱伦电影 曽我绫音-人妻調教爆乳I人妻 国内自拍六十七页 久热国内自拍 鲁鲁影院伦理 自拍在线超碰 AVT迅雷 番号大全 mp4 国产自拍 看片 欧州人体配种视频 白嫩小美女汤圆 AV 老外口交视频在线播放 分分日在视频下载 福利国模视频在线 免费 自拍偷拍初中鲍鱼粉 资源站在线视频播放 120发连续中出集锦手机在线 好屌妞精品偷拍视频 97人人操在线视频 赤井美月 大喷水无码种子 亚洲人人色 都市乱伦; 大洋马磁力链接 下载 妻中密影音先锋资 五月天色欲阁 类似莉莉影院 播色屋官网 巴巴鱼秋霞影视 午夜快乐播 阁夜香做爱动态图 黄色网站在线看蕾姆 男女啪啪啪无住碍漫画 西瓜影院福利社 第四色免费电 传奇影院关于宅 97婷婷色秋霞影院 成人午夜福2000级 蝌蚪剧场 5xdd1com 神马 老子久草在线 欧美成人性 天堂岛av大全 亚洲天啪 尘欲香欲缠双 300mium-086 夜剧场 福利视频 西瓜影音 yinsebiship 福利aV 大学女友10p 大学生破处视频 自拍 东方va在线观看青青 免费做爱香蕉视频 男女在床上日逼不穿内衣内裤一级片三级片a片 黄片 罗莉 啊…用力…快点…好深影院 日本免费a片观看网站 美图赚赚视频福利在线 视频直播百度网盘 日空姐免费观看 youjizz动漫视频 伦理福利5400 韶姬胡萝卜在线看 周末情趣影院 精品福利漫画 上原亚衣在线高清 水萝拉家庭教师 mp4 温柔的谎言 磁力 日本动漫 搜一搜 欧美性,交小片 5x社区韩国女主播 大香蕉视频高清无吗 wwwdydognet127001 素人街搭音乐教师在哪里能看 成人 国产 自拍网站 少妇偷拍点播在线 卵蛋网我差点 女孩影院 西瓜影音 大香蕉一到九色 新井佑美 在线播放 天色综合av sao250con 1900 贺立有和刘宝玉操逼出水视频免费体检区 色偷拍自怕亚洲在线97 明星资源视频 56qao免费视频视频 萝莉你懂的2019 射精管理机器人 asmr 久久巨乳在线看 与单位女同事宾馆开房偷情 百度云福利微视频 青欲乐视频 久久影院se 月经期的黄色视频裸体交配 aiss—速度福利网 欧养在线i免费视频 女孩被强暴 手指9191 东京热,一本道免费无码 h里番网站 三级片韩国叫床 大屁股女神叫声可射技术太棒了 在线 秋霞电影伦理伦理片瓜皮 999奇米4色在线观看 和尚精油按摩 精品500av导航 2019最新免费的av网址 快播牛逼叉 水岛津实 高潮 chinese tubeboy free 中国浮力视影院 校友会背叛她的订婚-2午夜影院 小老弟影 下载 爱爱电影90l 国产不卡毛片 2018在线看的视频你懂得 胔她屄流淫水 噜噜网噜噜巴噜噜色午夜影院 一本道久豹子影 超碰任你干免费在线 X网网址 国产手机在线福利小视频在线观看 酒色丁香激情人妻小说 午夜群交视频 暴乳女神 西瓜影院小黄片儿 最新艺术片 高义与孙靖 借种 壮汉 偷拍性爱磁力链接 BAAN-136番号 老师和学生腿上挂了安全套里番 gegekan在线看 甘榴影院 1琪琪网伦理片性之道 青青草原绿色华人免费观看 护士、老师、空姐被强奸视频系列 橘优花迅雷在线播放 998dh com大香蕉 操美女思瑞 韩国学生俊男靓女酒店骑马摇摆抽插 苍井空91链接 爆乳女神私人玩物 波多野结衣 种子 不穿内裤西瓜影音 兵兵淫视 爆乳日韩美女视频 xxxww日本 设为首页加入收藏图片区88 008精品视频在线 狐姆姆导航 欧美高清无码 bt种子 欧美日韩特区 手机在线观看美祢藤 艳色网站 琪琪原色原网站大全琪琪影院 三季第一页天天啪 激情片试看60秒 国产爱清大色网 sex特写系列视频 操逼视频在线观看大鸡巴 操逼2 qibishe saozi8t理论片 SNIS-326 magnet xt urn btih vip主播在线福利视频 福利偷拍老司机老狼 校园伦理在线 超碰视频资源免费高清人妻 成人网在线观看 欧美性爱图区 北非欧美性交视频A片免费看 古堡中的性事视频 艾小青福利视频 性感 强奸母亲磁力链接 师大艺术系校花与男友激情视频 纯洁 亚洲 巨乳外教 色女孩综合影院 kkbokk升级访问升级 欧美熟女内射视频 欧美淫荡人妻丝袜av电影 特黄的免费大片录像(俄罗斯) 强奸乱伦三级中文字小视频 俄罗斯男女性生话视频 2019亚洲国内无码偷拍视频 午夜影院成人一级片肉蒲团 一起去吃拉面吧三国影院 成人动漫淫乱电影网 日本三级床呻吟视频 美国欧美伦理43页视频 成人视频制造商 伦理 大片 波多野吉衣一本道DⅴD 操逼在线视频国语 操逼 视频 舔舔 波波兔 磁力 播播影院 sei 操小比视频 插18岁少女开饱视频 国产手机福利人人干 pao555 偷拍福利秒拍福频 让老公插自己小姊妹 骚女在床上搞那个的事的视频要看到屁股和生植气官 日本伦理片在线观看更多 在线国产自拍小姐草 www曰本黄色片 iesp569磁力 情侣偷拍26uuu 1042成人网 大香蕉av万色屋 2018最新av淘宝在线 亚洲港台操逼视频 影音先锋AVss 亚洲成人手机网站 强暴美女磁力链接 下载 能看印度aⅴ视频 美国萝莉黄片 污污污大香蕉 Xxx经典国语网 明日花做爱视频 神马动漫影视午夜免费观看 社区在线长视频播放 AVwanz00790 主播[萌黛] 粉嫩阴蒂 国产自拍youjizz 狠狠摞网友自拍 金八国 国产自拍-亚洲自拍在线观看 马女人与狗2在线播放 欧洲三级艳情在线观看 鲁鲁鲁日日视频免费 猫米大香蕉伊在线mp4 露胸贱视频软件 色色ojsfoe 洛阳小少妇在线视频 少女自慰视频久久 噜噜吧色在线视频 奇米影视盒三级 视频 激情五月bd韩国电影 混血美女自摸和男友激情啪啪自拍诱人呻吟福利视频 火山性爱在线 极品淫逼97影院 键盘福利导航第一 教室男朋友吃我奶好爽 青娱乐在线秒播 激情小说 轮奸处子 极品亚洲欧美成人影院 激情大尺度迷奸片段片段 一个更新快的色综合 种子猫番号库 福利点云视频 色美女免费的视频 a片黄片免费看 护士午夜福利影视 qiqizhongjiebandibabumifeibofang 做爱 小视频 www 69pao 网爆门广东金马国际旅行社张x茹 九尾狐狸m视频女仆在线 汤姆tom四虎 网红刘婷与快递员发生解压包密码 灰毛衣链接密码最新 后入大奶妹子 黑人来中国跟美妞说鸡巴超级大骚货主动约炮鸡巴大也是很好的一件事啊- 黑暗圣经高清无码 好日日中文字幕 后入式插屁眼儿视频 小姐和顾客链接 magnet 明日花绮罗有码视频 萝莉自慰流水福利 偷拍商场女厕所全景磁力链接 magnet wwwdxjav AEEN资源 偷拍自拍在线精品四虎 韩国xo激情插b视频 人体艺术免费菊花 偷拍明星磁力 magnet juseshiping 噜噜噜抽插射射射sex 秋霞伦理手机在一线看片 韩国偷拍视频免费观看 好吊操清平免费视频 姆啪啪 韩国黄主播免费观看视频 国产主播自拍磁力链接bt种子下载 男女肏屄小视频 色色999日韩偷拍写真 夏芽爱莉百度云 手机福利免费成人视频 火影忍者剧场版色色 动漫XXOO视频在线观看 郭思盈磁力 床戏后入直插 女人B脱毛视频 里番douluo123 日韩学生姝美 女三级伦理福利大香 美女扣b在线视频 黄色小漫 阿福av在线 狠狠色在线 天海翼lpz 862在线播放 www57w 猫咪av短视频怎么没了 欧洲亚洲在线成人 papa678 在线观看黄色电影大全 3pornstarmovies 神马影院骚播网 女同拉拉大合集本田岬 咸人免费免播放器视频 神马午夜初夏 北条麻妃丝袜全集ed2k 高清援交做爱视频 汤芳人艺体照 外国女人的比为什么总是没水 人体艺术摄影欧州版 小姨子的丝情袜意3p 经典三级1页电影 1级图片日韩 第一熟女考妣 女人的逼被前男人艹过我和她生出的宝宝会不会和她前男人长得有点像 欧美美毛穴图 把妹妹尻 诱惑人体艺术一级图片 操b色 性虐第1页小说 人体性交写真 最新美国十次啦 丝袜妈妈被狗 偷拍自拍激情农夫导航 磁力链接东京热 强奸乱片网页影音先锋 爱色电影网图片 WWW_570ZZ_COM 激情男操女过程 肥仔影音黄色网站 人体艺术图片动态 肏老女人电影 草逼集 欧美人像顶级大胆人体艺术